Tuesday, March 24, 2009
Separating the Spurious from the Genuine
Sunday, March 22, 2009
Lit review: Technology and Learning
Sunday, March 08, 2009
We Watch
I'm looking forward to this movie and I'm trying to keep my expectations in check. Movies aren't books. You can't make a good movie by using the original text as your shooting script. You have to make changes any time you transmediate material and I expect changes in this case. I'm hoping that the creative team was able to make a good movie and stay faithful in principle to the original work. I also know that no movie can match the experience of a fifteen year old me, walking to the comic book store to pick up the new issue of the graphic novel that changed the form.
Friday, March 06, 2009
Student Video Projects
Ann's theme word "challenge" kept cropping up all day yesterday and continues to be central today. I was greatly challenged by last night's Stats class. Last week and this week, I feel that I have a very tentative understanding of what we are doing. And there's a five percent chance that I have no clue. I don't quite grasp what we learn by correlating residuals. It feels like it's just out of reach.
Wednesday, March 04, 2009
Teacher Expectations
- Reyes, P., Scribner, J., & Scribner, A. (Eds.). (1999). Lessons from high-performing Hispanic schools: Creating learning communities. New York: Teachers College Press.
- Weinstein, R., Gregory, A., & Strambler, M. (2004). Intractable self-fulfilling prophecies: Brown v Board of Education. American Psychologist, 59, 511-520.
- Pianta, R., Howes, C., Burchinal, M., Bryant, D., Clifford, R., Early, D., & Barbarin, O. (2004). Features of pre-kindergarten programs, classrooms, and teachers: Do they predict observed classroom quality and child-teacher interaction? Manuscript submitted for publication.
- Noguera, P. (2003). Schools, prisons, and social implications of punishment: Rethinking disciplinary practices. Theory into Practice, 42, 341-350.
- Frericks, A. (1974, March). Labeling of students by prospective teachers. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Convention, Chicago. [According to Jones & Jones, Frericks found that teachers viewing a group of students that they were told was low performing described their behaviors more negatively than when they were told they were "regular" students.]
- Spencer-Hall, D. (1981). Looking behind the teacher's back. Elementary School Journal, 81, 281-289.
- Sadker, D., & Sadker, M. (1985). Is the o.k. classroom o.k.? Phi Delta Kappan, 66, 358-361. [Teachers treating boys and girls differently.]
- Kahle, J. (1990). Why girls don't know. In M. Rowe (Ed.), What research says to the science teacher: The process of knowing. Washington, DC: National Science Testing Association.
- Lee, V. (1991, August). Sexism in single-sex and co-educational secondary school classrooms. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Sociological Association, Cincinnati, OH.
- American Association of University Women Educational Foundation. (1992). The AAUW report: How schools shortchange girls. Washington, DC: National Education Association.
- Cooper, H., & Good, T. (1983). Pygmalion grows up. New York: Longman. [Ways teachers treat low achievers differently]
- Brophy, J. (1983). Research on the self-fulfilling prophecy and teacher expectations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 631-661.
- Use information from tests, cumulative folders, and other teachers very carefully.
- Be flexible in your use of grouping strategies.
- Make sure all the students are challenged.
- Be especially careful about how you respond to low-achieving students during class discussions.
- Use materials that show a wide range of ethnic groups.
- Be fair in evaluation and disciplinary procedures.
- Communicate to all students that you believe they can learn - and mean it.
- Involve all students in learning tasks and in privileges.
- Monitor your nonverbal behavior.
Claude Steele was interviewed about his research on the same episode of Radio Lab. He has looked at the effects of expectation on the performance of African Americans and on the performance of women in testing situations. His work is listed here. Here's a recent reference to his work:
Aronson, J. & Steele, C. M. Stereotypes and the fragility of academic competence, motivation, and self-concept, (chapter to appear in A. Elliot & C. Dweck (Eds.) The Handbook of Competence.
I'd like to do some work in these areas, or at the very least, keep up with what these researchers are doing.
Go More
Tuesday, March 03, 2009
WoW
Heroes?
Monday, March 02, 2009
My day
Saturday, February 21, 2009
Things I Love Right Now
- Deviled crabs from Brocatto's in Ybor City. Fantastic.
- Weck's Deli in Land'O'Lakes. The Bailey Avenue sandwich is wonderful.
- Battlestar Gallactica. Holy cow, that's good stuff. Only a few episodes left and I have my theories...
- Lost. I've stuck with the show, even when I hated what they were doing with it, but I'm loving it right now. And I have my theories...
- Oscar Movies. We've seen Vicky, Cristina, Barcelona, Changeling, Frozen River, and Revolutionary Road in the past week. We've seen all of the noms for Best Pic, Direction, and all of the acting awards, except Robert Downey Jr. in Tropic Thunder and all of the screenplay nominees except for In Bruges. I love movies and I love sharing them Michael.
Tuesday, February 17, 2009
More Thoughts on Formative Design
Monday, February 16, 2009
Formative design
Tuesday, February 10, 2009
Pragmatic Use of Reading Models
That means that, although texts are intertextual and meanings are conditional and provisional, simpler models of reading may be appropriate, depending on the context, the audience, and the purpose. It also means that as researchers we are free to use a cognitive model of reading when the situation calls for it, even if we believe a strict cognitive model to be inaccurate. All models of the reading process are inaccurate. They are all imperfect approximations. Still, all models of reading aren't equivalent or equally supported by systematic inquiry. It's just that, incomplete as it is, a cognitive metaphor, for instance, may be the most efficacious approach in a given situation. It's not ideological agnosticism, but it's something like methodological pragmatism.
Defining 'Text'
"The text has been rendered by intertextually-informed research in two ways. Both, influenced considerably by literary theory and semiotics, require an altered notion of what constitutes a text. Although we usually think of the text as the object one reads-a textbook, a section of a passage, or the alphanumeric code printed on a page-it need not be confined to the boundaries of printed language. A text includes both linguistic and nonlinguistic signs. A text can be an utterance, a gesture, a thought, a structure, a function, or a piece of art, music, or drama (Rowe, 1987; Short. 1986; Siegel, 1984); in this more inclusive sense, a text is any sign that communicates meaning."
with the end note:
Here's the full ref:
Hartman, D. (1992). Intertextuality and Reading: The Text, the Reader, the Author, and the Context. Linguistics and Education, 4, 295-311.
And those other refs:
Rowe, D.W. (1987). Literacy learning as an intettextual process. In J.E. Readence & R.S. Baldwin (Eds.). Research in literacy: Merging perspectives, Thirty-sixth Yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp. lOl- 1 12). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.
Short, K.G. (1986). Literacy as a collaborative experience. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Indiana University, Bloomington.
Siegel, M.G. (1984). Reading as signification. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, Bloomington.
Monday, February 09, 2009
Bates 2004, Language deficits across groups
- Kids with a variety of impairments experience the same sequence and types of problems, although at different rates. The metaphor suggested is that the problem space, English, is like a highway. These kids are all stuck in the same slow lane.
- Young kids who have either RHD or LHD usually move into the normal range in elementary school. They seem to "catch up" somewhere between the ages of 3 and 5.
- Kids with Williams syndrome perform language tasks that are in keeping with their mental age. This contradicts other research that suggests that those with Williams syndrome are capable of complex grammatical constructions and sophisticated speech far beyond their levels of cognition. The claims of language savant status for Williams syndrome kids would have been used to challenge the notion of cognitive prerequisites for language.
- More research should be done using subjects with 'normal' language abilities placed under stressful processing situations in an attempt to simulate language processing disorders.
- More longitudinal studies across the language acquisition period.
- The need to recognize a new metaphor for language development. Rather than a Swiss Army knife, filled with specialized tools for specific situations, Bates compares the language-enabled brain to a giraffe's neck -- adapted to serve a new function (reaching high leaves) while retaining its older functions (turning the head, passing air and food). Bates refers to the "Functional infrastructure for language" chart to show older adaptations combining to provide the basis for language.
Wednesday, February 04, 2009
Podcast Podcast
Sunday, February 01, 2009
Vellutino & Schatschneider 2004
Vellutino, F., & Schatschneider, C. (2004). Experimental and quasi-experimental design in literacy research. In (chapter 7) N. Duke, & M. Mallette, editors. Literacy Research Methodologies. New York: Guilfords.
(aka Duke Ch. 7)
These authors did an outstanding job of summarizing and contextualizing the issues and parameters surrounding experimental and quasi-experimental design. I agree with Melissa that this chapter should be required reading in Stats. I think the difference here is in perspective. These writers place statistical concepts and procedures firmly within the context of literacy research. In Stats texts, I think the presentation of the material concentrates on the details without the context - and that's probably reasonable since we are learning concepts and procedures that are common to many disciplines and our classmates are not all interested in literacy research. However, that is a good argument for a quantitative statistical measurement class taught specifically for literacy researchers within the Reading program.
Vellutino & Schatschneider propose evaluation of experiments and quasi-experiments based on the adequacy of the hypothetical counterfactual and how well the design addresses concerns about internal validity, external validity, statistical conclusion validity, and construct validity. They give examples of how one might evaluate research using their own studies as exemplars. They make it clear that all studies have weaknesses and flaws and that understanding the flaws is important to understanding how to connect and apply research beyond the contexts of the original studies.
I think that this chapter (and its references) will be useful for evaluating existing research and designing new studies.
This Week's Reading
Historical Research Duke 7
Content Analysis Kamil 3
History of Reading, NRC 2007
Monaghan 2007
Oral History, King and Stahl
Stahl & King, History
Stahl (by King)
EDF 7265
Tomasello, M. & Brooks, P.J. (1999). Early syntactic development: A Construction Grammar approach (pp. 161-190). In M. Barrett (ed.), The Development of Language. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press Ltd.
EDF 7408 Stats 2
S: Ch 4; C&S: Ch 7
Wednesday, January 28, 2009
Class notes, Psych of Language Development
- Constraints/Principles Theories (think about Piaget)
- Noun-category bias - Quinean connundrum
- Markman's Mutual Exclusivity
- Taxomonic Assumption
- Social-Pragmatic Theory (think about Vygotsky)
- Joint attention - social construction
- Associationistic Theory
- Emergentist Coalition Theory (different theories apply at different stages)
- Hybrid approach
Tuesday, January 27, 2009
Wagner & Torgesen, 1987
My assigned article is Wagner and Torgesen’s (1987) literature review on the influence of phonological processing on reading ability acquisition. My outside reading is Allington and Woodside-Jiron’s (1999) critique of the uses and misuses of research in shaping education policy.
This article is coauthored by Joseph Torgesen and Richard Wagner, both of whom are currently at the Florida Center for Reading Research (FCRR) at FSU. They have been very influential in shaping reading policy in the state of Florida and nationally for the past several years. To add to my understanding of this article, I chose an article by Richard Allington, an outspoken critic of educational policy that has resulted from the use, or misuse, of research like Wagner and Torgesen’s. Allington’s piece is not a direct response to Wagner & Torgesen (1987), but it challenges the findings and their policy implications nonetheless.
In this literature review, Wagner and Torgesen wanted to see if there was a causal relationship between phonological processing and learning reading skills. They reviewed three distinct bodies of research that corresponded to three distinct aspects of phonological processing:
- phonological awareness (or phonemic awareness). This is the ability to perceive phonemes, measured by the ability to segment and blend phonemes in isolation.
- phonological recoding in lexical access. This refers to translating a written word into speech in order to access its meaning.
- phonetic recoding to maintain information in working memory. This refers to coding words into phonemes for efficient storage in working memory for processing.
Wagner and Torgesen discuss the complications of trying to study questions of correlation and causation of such a complex and invisible process as reading. For example, two variables that seem related could both be influenced by a third variable that is not being measured. The researcher thinks she's looking at a cause and an effect, when she's only looking at two effects of the same unobserved cause. Likewise, there may be a web of unobserved causation at work in any study looking for correlation between variables, particularly when dealing with mental processes. Because direct observation of mental processes is not possible, a researcher may also confuse one variable with another, for instance unintentionally measuring working memory instead of, or in addition to, phonological ability. Likewise in experimental research, the researcher assumes that the training he provides to an experimental group is effectively targeting the skill or ability being studied. The degree to which this is true greatly affects the reliability of the results. The authors sought to offset the weaknesses of each type of study by combining studies of both types, longitudinal correlational studies and experimental studies, in their analysis.
Wagner and Torgesen find many areas of ambiguity and obscurity in the analysis of these data as it relates to their research question. They indicate areas within the reviewed studies where different methods would have answered a particular question, but the answer is unavailable because of the methods actually used. In every case, they describe what an ideal study to answer a particular question would look like, then describe the data from the studies that they found.
The authors found that the three aspects of phonological processing discussed in this article seem to be three ways of measuring the same underlying process. In addition, despite the methodological incompatibilities between Wagner and Toregesen’s research question and the data sets available, they reached the following conclusions:
The article that I selected by Allington & Woodside-Jiron was written more than a decade later in response to a widely-circulated white paper (Thirty Years of Research: What We Now Know About How Children Learn to Read) that purported to summarize best practices for classroom reading instruction. As the authors detailed, the research cited in the white paper did not adequately support the conclusions drawn. This white paper concerned these researchers because it strongly influenced educational policy in multiple states.
Although they don’t specifically address Wagner & Torgesen (1987), Allington and Woodside-Jiron criticize the types of conclusions drawn by Wagner and Torgesen. Specifically, the “studies have more often produced reliable, replicable gains for a specialized population only on measures of phonological processing and psuedo-word pronunciation tasks, while reliable, replicable gains on word reading, fluency, and prose comprehension have been more difficult to generate” (p. 3).
Allington & Woodside-Jiron offer the interpretation that studies of phonological processing provide evidence that: 1) 15-20% of students experience problems with phonological processing; 2) these problems are associated with early reading acquisition; and 3) difficulties in phonological awareness can be remedied. According to Allington & Woodside-Jyron, that’s it. Causation has not been established. There is insufficient evidence to make sweeping changes to instruction of entire school populations based on the research available. Research is often misappropriated in support of policy goals. These authors charge that the researchers involved benefit from having their research used as the basis for policy decisions and policy-makers benefit by adding the “stamp of approval” of scientific research to their policies. Simply put, “the use of 'research' as a policy advocacy tool seems less dependent on the reliability of synthesis of the research than on the ability to place 'research' summaries that support particular policy agendas into the hands of advocates and policymakers." (p. 11).
When reading research, I think it’s important to adopt a critical perspective not only toward sampling procedures and effect sizes, but toward the personalities and the politics connected to the research. In a perfect world, research would represent a dispassionate search for the truth. Considering the educational policy directions of the past twenty years, research like Wagner and Torgesen’s must be considered in a broader, and more critical, context.
Allington, R., & Woodside-Jiron, H. (1999). The politics of literacy teaching: How "research" shaped educational policy. Educational Researcher, 28 (8), 4-13. Retrieved from: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1176311
Monday, January 26, 2009
This week's reading for Wednesday
Goswami, U. (2001). Early phonological development and the acquisition of literacy (pp. 111-125). In S.B. Neuman & D.K. Dickinson (eds.) Handbook of Early Literacy Research. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. (everyone)(Pro-Copy)
Wagner, R.K. & Torgesen, J.K., (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 192-212.
Plus, something that relates to Wagner and Torgesen, and presentation on that. I'd like to do that presentation in Prezi.
Class notes, Reading Research
Kinch says, the higher the proposition density, the harder it is to understand a text.
Protocol analysis is inductive, a window into the cognitive process. You are making inferences to construct a model the cognitive process, a model of a reader's thoughts.
Subjects are trained in protocol analysis before data collection with similar task using different material. During training, reinforce process talk, not product talk. By doing this, you are skewing the verbal protocol toward the type of data you are looking for. If you don't train subjects, you can get a lot of unusable data. When you do, you run the risk of not finding things that you aren't looking for.
Subjects have to be engaging in goal-directed behavior so that you can collect data; they have to be trying to do something, not just read without a specific purpose.
The researcher's verbal prompts need to be on topic. Talking about making a hamburger while cleaning a carburetor will disrupt the cognitive process. In protocol analysis, you talk about the task that you are doing; it doesn't disrupt, it just slows down cognitive processing.
Just enough categories to account for the data. Start broad, with many categories, then check back with the data to narrow.
Next week, historical analysis.
Harvey Graff looks at what literacy does to cultures. He sees literacy's outside impact on culture, separate from culture. Doug Hartman and Jennifer Monahan research the history of reading. Also, content analysis and document analysis. Jim King and Norm Stahl work with oral histories. We will be looking at USF's collection of educator oral histories on iTunes U. Lastly, we will be looking at how one situates the history of reading for course delivery.
Wednesday, January 21, 2009
Class notes, week 3
Language depends on a prior development of communication. A desire to communicate must precede that.
Word meaning is arbitrary and idiosyncratic.
Prelinguistic dialogue
- situation dependent
- routines
What do children learn?
- Initiation & termination of conversations
- Turn-taking
- Pacing
- Verbal and non-verbal elements
- Birth to 6 mos. Socialization and Early Communication
- Newborn
- Interactions synchronized with speech
- Preference for human speech
- 1 Month
- Engaged in interactional sequences (movement and eye contact)
- Imitate pitch and duration of speech
- Develops social smile and cooing
- 2 Months
- Mouth movements are more distinct
- Infant develops eye contact with mother
- 3 Months
- Child likely to revocalize if caregiver responds verbally resulting in "conversational" turn-taking
- Helps develop babbling and turn-taking
- Babbling becomes speechlike (use of syllables)
- Protoconversations
- Rituals emerge
- Provide predictable patterns of behavior and speech
- Game playing emerges
- Include aspects of communication
- Child likely to revocalize if caregiver responds verbally resulting in "conversational" turn-taking
- 5 Months
- Deliberate imitation of movements and vocalizations
- Facial imitation most frequent b/w 4-6 mos
- Face to face play
- Infant exposed to facial expressions
- Vocalization based on temperament
- Deliberate imitation of movements and vocalizations
- 6 Months
- Interest in toyes and objects increase
- Eye-hand coordination increases
- Interactions include infant, caregiver, and object
- Joint attention begins to develop
- Initiated by caregiver
- Interest in toyes and objects increase
- 7 Months
- Begins developing attachment
- Demonstrates selective listening to simple words
- Complies with simple requests
- 8-10 Months
- Imitate simple motor behaviors
- waves bye
- Folllow maternal pointing and glancing
- Imitate simple motor behaviors
- Newborn